Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Altruism

Are Poorer People More Generous?

3 ways wealth affects generosity.

Key points

  • Global economic growth has been fairly consistent for the last two centuries.
  • People with more money are not always more generous.
  • People who feel richer sometimes give less than those who feel poorer.

With the ongoing economic impacts of the pandemic and rising inflation around the world, many people have less money to spend. However, global economic growth has been surprisingly consistent for the last two centuries. Understanding the impact of these societal changes in wealth is a pressing issue for economists, psychologists, sociologists and other researchers.

How wealthy people are, or how wealthy they think they are, can have big implications for how we interact with others. One important finding is that wealth has been linked to what researchers call ‘prosocial behaviours’, actions that include helping others and being generous with our money or time. It seems intuitive that people who have more money are more likely to donate money to others, but not all studies find that this is the case. There are also many diverse ways to help others that do not involve money, and different ways of measuring wealth which complicate the picture.

Solving the puzzle of how wealth impacts prosocial behaviour, or if there is no impact at all, is essential right now. To alleviate international conflict or climate change, people around the world will have to engage in prosocial behaviours that benefit others. But how exactly does wealth affect prosocial behaviour, and are those who are wealthier more or less generous?

Cottonbro/Pexels
Generosity has been linked to wealth
Source: Cottonbro/Pexels

Objective wealth and generosity

An important distinction can be made between objective wealth—metrics such as household income at the individual level or the wealth of a country—and what we might call subjective wealth—how wealthy we perceive ourselves compared to others in our society. Over the past few years, several studies have reported that those with higher objective wealth are the ones that are more prosocial. For example, one study examined the association between household income and prosocial behaviour around the world. Prosocial behaviour was measured by asking around 80,000 participants across 76 countries a mixture of survey questions such as, “How willing are you to give to good causes without expecting anything in return?” These hypothetical questions were shown in a separate sample to predict people’s behaviour in experiments with real money. The study found that those who had a higher household income also scored higher in prosociality.

Another study examined the effects of wealth at the broader level of wealth within a neighbourhood. They measured wealth objectively with a score based on income and employment. Prosocial behaviour was willingness to post a lost letter, return a dropped item, or stop to let someone cross the road, so these behaviours did not cost any money. Neighbourhood wealth predicted willingness to engage in prosocial behaviour with higher wealth neighbourhoods engaging in prosocial behaviour more frequently.

However, the findings between higher income and more giving are not always consistent. For example, some researchers showed that those with a lower household income would give more to a partner in a trust game, where participants are asked to give some money to a partner and the partner can choose to return some of it for a bonus. There is also evidence that people in poorer countries were more willing to donate to a hypothetical charity than those in richer countries, particularly if the charity would help people in their own country specifically. Further research is needed to understand why there can be differences between studies in whether wealthier people give more.

Subjective wealth and generosity

When we think of wealth it is natural to think just of how much money a person has. However, research has shown that how wealthy we feel rather than how much we have is a really important factor. Subjective wealth, or how wealthy you perceive yourself to be relative to others in society, can be measured in many ways. One popular way is to use the MacArthur ladder scale. For this measurement, people view a drawing of a ladder with 10 rungs and imagine that the ladder represents where people stand in society. They read: “At the top of the ladder are the people who are the best off, those who have the most money, most education, and best jobs. At the bottom are the people who are the worst off, those who have the least money, least education, worst jobs, or no job. Please place an ‘X’ on the rung that best represents where you think you stand on the ladder.”

Source: sparqtools
The MacArthur ladder scale can be used to measure subjective wealth
Source: sparqtools

Using this measure another global study, this time measuring over 40,000 people in 67 countries, found that those who perceived themselves to be lower on the ladder scale gave more in a hypothetical donation task. So lower subjective wealth predicted greater generosity. Consistent with this finding, another study found that those people who subjectively perceived themselves to be a lower economic rank in society gave more in a donation game. Together these studies show that unlike objective wealth, for subjective wealth it might be that those who perceive themselves as poorer give more.

Scarcity and generosity

Another important piece of the puzzle is the effect of having very limited levels of resources, which can be termed ‘scarcity’ or ‘precarity’. In these cases, it could be that any links between prosocial behaviour and subjective or objective wealth break down. So the association between prosocial behaviour and wealth would not always be positive or always negative, but instead there would be a more complicated pattern where those who are the very wealthiest or the very poorest behave differently from those with middling levels of subjective or objective wealth. Some researchers have argued for the more complicated pattern and pointed to research in other species suggesting that in general scarcity increases prosociality but only when scarcity is not extreme. Indeed, social relationships are very important in times of upheaval or stress. For example, during the COVID-19 pandemic social support was shown to be an important buffer with those with more social connections reporting reduced stress and fatigue. Future research will be important to understand the association between wealth and generosity as wealth continues to change around the world but the need for prosocial behaviour remains.

References

Cutler, J., Nitschke, J. P., Lamm, C., & Lockwood, P. L. (2021). Older adults across the globe exhibit increased prosocial behavior but also greater in-group preferences. Nature Aging, 1(10), 880–888.

Kosse, F., & Tincani, M. M. (2020). Prosociality predicts labor market success around the world. Nature Communications, 11(1), 5298.

Lazarus, J. (2017). Cooperation in adversity: an evolutionary approach. Global Discourse, 7(4), 571–598.

Nitschke, J. P., Forbes, P. A., Ali, N., Cutler, J., Apps, M. A., Lockwood, P. L., & Lamm, C. (2021). Resilience during uncertainty? Greater social connectedness during COVID‐19 lockdown is associated with reduced distress and fatigue. British Journal of Health Psychology, 26(2), 553-569.

Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771–784.

Stamos, A., Lange, F., Huang, S. C., & Dewitte, S. (2020). Having less, giving more? Two preregistered replications of the relationship between social class and prosocial behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 84, 103902.

Zwirner, E., & Raihani, N. (2020). Neighbourhood wealth, not urbanicity, predicts prosociality towards strangers. Proceedings. Biological Sciences / The Royal Society, 287(1936), 20201359.

advertisement
More from Patricia Lockwood, Ph.D., and Jo Cutler, Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today